Poisoning of honey bees (Apis mellifera) by sodium fluoroacetate (1080) in baits

Similar documents
FEEDING BEES * G.F. TOWNSEND, CANADA

The Courtship of Honeybees, Buckwheat and Watermelon Blossoms

Sampling for Varroa Mites and Treatment Thresholds

Evaluation of Caffeine and Garlic Oil as Bird Repellents

Psa and Italian Kiwifruit Orchards an observation by Callum Kay, 4 April 2011

27-Feb-16. Equipment Needed

COMPARISON OF CORE AND PEEL SAMPLING METHODS FOR DRY MATTER MEASUREMENT IN HASS AVOCADO FRUIT

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International General Certificate of Secondary Education

Presentation for: 2018 Eastern KY Beekeeping School Kevin Hale

EFFECTIVENESS OF THE INSECTICIDAL SMALL HIVE BEETLE REFUGE TRAP APITHOR IN REDUCING ADULT BEETLE NUMBERS IN BEE HIVES.

EFFECT OF CULTURAL MANIPULATION OF "MUMMY" WALNUTS ON WINTER SURVIVAL OF NAVEL ORANGEWORM

EFFECT OF TOMATO GENETIC VARIATION ON LYE PEELING EFFICACY TOMATO SOLUTIONS JIM AND ADAM DICK SUMMARY

PRACTICAL 12 MISCELLANEOUS MANAGEMENT (Dividing, uniting, queen management, supplementary feeding, shifting bee colonies, robbing, absconding)

HONEY. The Natural Sweetener. Cooperative Extension Service College of Agriculture University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Circular 1169

Hiving and Care of Packaged Bees

CORRELATIONS BETWEEN CUTICLE WAX AND OIL IN AVOCADOS

THE ANTISCORBUTIC VALUE OF FRESH AND CANNED ENGLISH TOMATOES. XC. (Received May 1st, 1924.)

Effects of Preharvest Sprays of Maleic Hydrazide on Sugar Beets

Michigan Grape & Wine Industry Council 2012 Research Report. Understanding foliar pest interactions for sustainable vine management

Project Concluding: Summary Report Mandarin Trial for the California Desert

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International General Certificate of Secondary Education

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International General Certificate of Secondary Education

Diseases in Illinois Seed Corn as Found in the Fifth Utility Corn Show

NE-1020 Cold Hardy Wine Grape Cultivar Trial

INFLUENCE OF THIN JUICE ph MANAGEMENT ON THICK JUICE COLOR IN A FACTORY UTILIZING WEAK CATION THIN JUICE SOFTENING

Flowering and Fruiting Morphology of Hardy Kiwifruit, Actinidia arguta

ALBINISM AND ABNORMAL DEVELOPMENT OF AVOCADO SEEDLINGS 1

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International General Certificate of Secondary Education

Potential of Vapormate TM for control of arthropod pests on citrus. Vapormate update

PERFORMANCE OF HYBRID AND SYNTHETIC VARIETIES OF SUNFLOWER GROWN UNDER DIFFERENT LEVELS OF INPUT

Research - Strawberry Nutrition

NEW ZEALAND AVOCADO FRUIT QUALITY: THE IMPACT OF STORAGE TEMPERATURE AND MATURITY

Chapter II MATERIALS AND METHOD

Effect of Storage Period and Ga3 Soaking of Bulbs on Growth, Flowering and Flower Yield of Tuberose (Polianthes Tuberosa L.) Cv.

Growing Fruits. Which fruits to choose

HARCOURT APPLE CIDER AGED VINEGAR. Made with Live Vinegar Mother EQUINE. Made from 100% Real Australian Apples Not from Concentrate

DEVELOPMENT AND STANDARDISATION OF FORMULATED BAKED PRODUCTS USING MILLETS

2014 Agrium AT Fertilizer Trial Glen R. Obear and Bill Kreuser, Ph.D University of Nebraska-Lincoln. Objectives

F&N 453 Project Written Report. TITLE: Effect of wheat germ substituted for 10%, 20%, and 30% of all purpose flour by

Solubility Lab Packet

Biocidal Product Families instead of Frame Formulations The right step forward? Sara Kirkham

Things We Need To Know About

Preliminary Studies on the Preservation of Longan Fruit in Sugar Syrup

TEMPERATURE CONDITIONS AND TOLERANCE OF AVOCADO FRUIT TISSUE

Presentation for: 2017 Eastern KY Beekeeping School Kevin Hale

Cambridge International Examinations Cambridge International General Certificate of Secondary Education

A.P. Environmental Science. Partners. Mark and Recapture Lab addi. Estimating Population Size

Buying Filberts On a Sample Basis

Influence of GA 3 Sizing Sprays on Ruby Seedless

Activity 2.3 Solubility test

Vineyard Insect Management what does a new vineyard owner/manager need to know?

GROWTH RATES OF RIPE ROT FUNGI AT DIFFERENT TEMPERATURES

Honeybees Late Fall Check

The Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois,

Notes on the Philadelphia Fed s Real-Time Data Set for Macroeconomists (RTDSM) Capacity Utilization. Last Updated: December 21, 2016

Fungicides for phoma control in winter oilseed rape

THE EFFECT OF GIRDLING ON FRUIT QUALITY, PHENOLOGY AND MINERAL ANALYSIS OF THE AVOCADO TREE

Mastering Measurements

Peaches & Nectarines and Cherry Annual Reports

ESTIMATING ANIMAL POPULATIONS ACTIVITY

7. LOCALIZATION OF FRUIT ON THE TREE, BRANCH GIRDLING AND FRUIT THINNING

MONITORING WALNUT TWIG BEETLE ACTIVITY IN THE SOUTHERN SAN JOAQUIN VALLEY: OCTOBER 2011-OCTOBER 2012

Managing Spotted Wing Drosophila, Drosophila Suzukii Matsumara, In Raspberry.

Wood Sugar Molasses. for Dairy Cattle. I. R. Jones. gricu kural Experiment Station. State College Station Circular 181. regon. September 1949.

In the preparation of this Tanzania Standard assistance was derived from:

Draft Indian Standard SPICES AND CONDIMENTS - CORIANDER, WHOLE AND GROUND - SPECIFICATION (Third Revision)

**If meat is over two inches in thickness and or has bone structures, use a meat injector to inject the brine solution into the meat.

SELF-POLLINATED HASS SEEDLINGS

Problem How does solute concentration affect the movement of water across a biological membrane?

Effects of Different Packaging Materials on the Shelf Stability of Ginger Juice

Relationship between Mineral Nutrition and Postharvest Fruit Disorders of 'Fuerte' Avocados

Grower Summary TF 170. Plums: To determine the performance of 6 new plum varieties. Annual 2012

COMPARATIVE POPULATION DYNAMICS OF TROPILAELAPS CLAREAE AND VARROA JACOBSONI MITES ON HONEYBEES J. WOYKE 1

Experiment 2: ANALYSIS FOR PERCENT WATER IN POPCORN

GARDENING WEEK 9 EXTENDING THE LIFE OF YOUR GARDEN: FOOD PRESERVATION AND SEED SAVING

w w w. s o p i b. c o m Sulphate of Potash and Wine Grapes

WINE GRAPE TRIAL REPORT

Observations of the Baka huntergatherers in two controlled foraging trips in the tropical rainforest of southeastern Cameroon

PERFORMANCE OF FOUR FORAGE TURNIP VARIETIES AT MADRAS, OREGON, J. Loren Nelson '

Progress Report on Avocado Breeding

QUARTERLY REVIEW OF THE PERFORMANCE OF THE DAIRY INDUSTRY 1

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION

Let s Preserve. Tomatoes. Bulletin #4085. Recommended Varieties. Quantity. Table 1. Quantities of Fresh Tomatoes Needed for Tomato Products

Studies in the Postharvest Handling of California Avocados

DEVELOPMENT OF MILK AND CEREAL BASED EXTRUDED PRODUCTS

Quality of Canadian oilseed-type soybeans 2016

Bounty71 rootstock an update

Where in the Genome is the Flax b1 Locus?

Project Justification: Objectives: Accomplishments:

Angel Rebollar-Alvitar and Michael A. Ellis The Ohio State University/OARDC Department of Plant Pathology 1680 Madison Avenue Wooster, OH 44691

INNOVATIVE SOLUTIONS POWERING YOUR SAFETY SUCCESS

Tomato Product Cutting Tips

Don t wash raw chicken. Campylobacter: a concern for us all

5 Populations Estimating Animal Populations by Using the Mark-Recapture Method

Presentation for: 2018 Eastern KY Beekeeping School Kevin Hale

The Cruel Exploiter- Acacia confusa (Taiwan Acacia)

A Practical Guide to Biocidal Products and Articles

Classification and Botanical Description of Imported Varieties of Hops (Humulus lupulus) in Nelson, New Zealand

Mischa Bassett F&N 453. Individual Project. Effect of Various Butters on the Physical Properties of Biscuits. November 20, 2006

The Positive List System in Japan and Our Approach to the Issues of Pesticide Residues in Cocoa

Transcription:

New Zealand Journal of Zoology ISSN: 31-4223 (Print) 1175-8821 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tnzz2 Poisoning of honey bees (Apis mellifera) by sodium fluoroacetate (18) in baits R. M. Goodwin & A. Ten Houten To cite this article: R. M. Goodwin & A. Ten Houten (1991) Poisoning of honey bees (Apis mellifera) by sodium fluoroacetate (18) in baits, New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 18:1, 45-51, DOI: 1.18/314223.1991.1757947 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/1.18/314223.1991.1757947 Published online: 22 Nov 213. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 11 View related articles Citing articles: 3 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalinformation?journalcode=tnzz2 Download by: [6.234.11.221] Date: 28 February 216, At: 2:16

New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 1991, Vol. 18: 45-51 31-4223/91/181-45$2.5/ Crown copyright 1991 45 Poisoning of honey bees (Apis me/litera) by sodium fluoroacetate (1 8) in baits Downloaded by [6.234.11.221] at 2:16 28 February 216 R. M. GOODWIN A. TEN HOUTEN Ruakura Apicultural Research and Advisory Unit Ministry of Agriculture and Fisheries Ruakura Agricultural Centre Private Bag, Hamilton, New Zealand Abstract The behaviour of honey bees feeding on sodium fluoroacetate-jam baits, which are used for opossum control, was investigated to determine the reasons for reported honey bee kills. Honey bees were shown to feed readily on 18-jam baits when they were presented outside their hives. There were no debilitating effects for up to 2 h after feeding on the baits, which allowed them to make up to four foraging trips to the baits and recruit additional foragers. Black strap molasses was shown to have a repellent effect on honey bees when mixed with the jam baits. Foragers were able to detect differences in concentration of as little as 5%. Twenty percent molasses was found to repel inexperienced foragers strongly; however, they could be trained to accept higher concentrations. Two percent oxalic acid (a component of molasses) was found to repel honey bees when added to 2M sugar syrup or jam baits. Molasses-jam baits and jam baits appeared to be equally attractive to opossums when tested in the field. These results suggest that molasses or oxalic acid may could be incorporated with 18-jam baits to prevent honey bees feeding on them. Keywords honey bees; poisoning; sodium fluoroacetate; repellents; molasses; oxalic acid Z933 Received 12 September 199; accepted 3 January 1991 INTRODUCTION Sodium fluoroacetate (18) is used in New Zealand to control opossums (Trichosurus vulpecula). It is added to either jam or carrot baits. The jam baits contain 18 at a concentration of.5%,.8%, or.15% w.w. and are dyed green (Permicol Green 2-6794 dye) to protect birds. Cinnamon is added to mask the flavour of the 18 poison (Morgan 1985). After closely monitoring poisoning programs over 6 years, Mcintosh et al. (1964) were unable to find evidence of mass mortality of honey bees (Apis mellifera) and concluded that jam baits did not endanger honey bees or effect honey. However, large bee kills have been reported recently in areas where opossum poisoning programmes have been conducted. Of four samples of dead honey bees tested, three have been found to contain 18 (3.1, 3.8, and 1 mg/kg bees, unpublished test results). The high bee mortalities reported recently may be caused by the poison jam formulation having been changed since the early trials (Mcintosh et al. 1964). The raspberry jam has been replaced with apple, the green dye is now a legal requirement, the wide divergences in toxicity of the bait have been eliminated, and cinnamon has been added along with 8 ppm potassium sorbate anti-fungal preservative (Logan pers. comm. 1987). The LD 5 for technical grade sodium fluoroacetate in.2 ml of sucrose solution fed to honey bees is.8 Jlg (Palmer-Jones 1957). However, those tests bear little relationship to the actual toxicity of 18 to foraging honey bees because the bees were starved and then permitted to consume a full dose of the 18-sucrose solution. Those that regurgitated any of the solution were discarded. Under field conditions, a foraging honey bee collecting the jam baits would not be starved first and therefore would not imbibe as much 18 as a starved bee. A starved bee would presumably also take more syrup from its crop into its stomach than a bee foraging on 18-jam baits. Also, foragers would regurgitate most of their load when returning to their hive and would only consume small amounts of any 18-jam they collected. It is

46 New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 1991, Vol. 18 Downloaded by [6.234.11.221] at 2:16 28 February 216 c as - as.q - C'l as -c () ll. 1 5 5 1 1 5 2 25 Percentage molasses in bait tria I II 2 II 3 Fig. I Percentage of molasses-jam baits taken, over 7 h, with increasing molasses concentrations (all concentrations at the same feeding station). The lowest concentration was removed for each consecutive trial. c 1 co -- as.q - C'l as -c () 5 O+-'-.J..,.-t--"-':...:...t:...:...t:...L..<:...::--"-'::...4..,,...1'-.L......:...t.-'-'- 1 2 3 4 5 Percentage molasses in bait 3 Fig. 2 Percentage of molassesjam baits taken, over 7 h, with increasing molasses concentration (only one concentration at each feeding station). The vertical lines are standard errors of 9 replicates. thus possible that they could collect 18-jam and pass it to house bees without receiving a lethal dose. At present, operators are instructed not to lay baits closer than 4 m from apiary sites to reduce the possibility of honey bees feeding on the baits. The choice of a 4 m limit does not however appear to be based on any scientific evidence. The 4 m limit was devised when all poisoning was carried out during June and July. Poisoning programmes are

Goodwin & Ten Houten-Poisoning of honey bees 47 Downloaded by [6.234.11.221] at 2:16 28 February 216 Fig. 3 Percentage of oxalic acid-!:: Q) 1 sugar syrup taken, over 1 h, with increasing oxalic acid concentration C13 (all concentrations at the same - feeding station). The vertical lines c. are standard errors of 6 replicates. ::J... ->- Ul 5 Q) Cl C13 -!:: Q)... Q) now also carried out in the spring and early autumn when honey bees forage more actively. Registered beekeepers are usually notified of any poisoning programmes and are asked to remove their hives while the poisoning is carried out. The aim of this investigation is to describe the behaviour of honey bees presented with 18-jam baits and to determine whether molasses or oxalic acid could be used to repel honey bees from baits. Uematsu et al. 1985 reported that honey bees find molasses to be relatively unattractive. Honey solutions containing -1% molasses were accepted by foraging honey bees whereas those containing higher concentrations of molasses were less acceptable. As molasses is frequently used as stock feed and has been incorporated in opossum baits previously it is likely to be attractive to opossums. Trials conducted with molasses incorporated with pellets have shown that the addition of molasses does not effect the bait's attractiveness to opossums (Morgan 1985; Morgan pers. comm.). Oxalic acid, along with citric, malic, and glycolic acids make up between 1-5% of molasses (percentage weight) (Meade & Chen 1977). However, there is nothing in the literature to suggest that oxalic acid is repellent to honey bees. The only references to oxalic acid and honey bees is for its use in the control of varroa mites. MATERIALS AND METHODS The first trials were to determine if foragers would visit jam baits, and how long foragers survived after a...2.5 2 5 Percentage oxalic acid in sugar syrup consuming 18 from baits when they have the opportunity to pass most of it to house bees on returning to their hive. This was extended to determine how many foraging trips honey bees can make to baits before they succumb to the 18. Further trials were conducted to ascertain if molasses or oxalic acid (a component of molasses) could be used to repel honey bees from jam baits. Exploitation of jam baits Trial 1. Ten jam baits were laid around a group of three hives to determine whether honey bees would forage from jam baits. The jam baits were apple based (including the skin and core but excluding the pips) with 35% sugar and 3% water. The baits were approximately 2 m from the colonies. They were then checked every hour to determine the number that were being visited by foraging honey bees. Trial 2. The survival rate and survival time of honey bees making one foraging trip to a 18-jam bait was investigated by training honey bees from a two-frame observation hive to feed from a non-toxic jam bait laid about 3 m from the hive. The jam bait was then exchanged for a 18-jam bait. About 2 foragers were permitted to feed on the 18-jam bait. They were then marked individually with water colour paints while feeding on the baits. Each forager was only permitted to visit the 18-jam bait once. The bait was removed when 2 foragers had been marked or when the first marked forager returned for a second trip. The marked foragers were observed each hour inside the observation hive over the next 6 h and again 24 h later. A record was kept of the number of

48 c: Cl) 1 CIS -.'!:::: CIS.c New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 1991, Vol. 18 Fig. 4 Percentage of oxalic acidjam baits taken over 7 h with increasing oxalic acid concentration (only one concentration at each feeding station). The vertical lines are standard errors of 1 replicates. - 5 Downloaded by [6.234.11.221] at 2:16 28 February 216 Cl) Cl CIS -c: Cl) "- olkj22zzl.2.5 1 2 5 Percentage oxalic acid in bait marked foragers that died over this time. This was repeated four times over several weeks until a total of 79 foragers were tested. Trial 3. The number of foraging trips that could be made to a 18-jam bait was determined by training foragers to a jam bait laid 3 m from a hive. The jam bait was then exchanged for a 18-jam bait Twenty foragers were permitted to feed from the bait. These foragers were individually marked. A record was kept of the number of return trips each forager made to the bait. Repellency of blackstrap molasses Trial I. Jam baits were made up that contained (W: W), 5, 1, 15, 2, 25, and 3% blackstrap molasses. Honey bees were trained to a feeding station 2 m from their colony. Sugar syrup was placed at the feeding station each morning. When large numbers (>1) of bees were feeding, the syrup was replaced with the jam and molasses-jam baits. The baits (approx. 4 g) were placed in small plastic petri-dishes and weighed. Four replicates of each concentration were arranged randomly at the feeding station at about 9 h. The baits were removed and weighed between 16 and 17 h to determine the amount of the baits consumed. The first trial also consisted of a set of controls to determine weight loss caused by evaporation. The controls consisted of one bait of each concentration covered by a wire cage to prevent honey bees gaining access. The trial was repeated on five consecutive days. The lowest concentration was removed from each successive trial until only 2, 25, and 3% molasses remained, to determine if foragers could be trained to accept baits with high molasses concentrations. Trial 2. The attractiveness of molasses-jam baits to inexperienced foragers was tested by establishing five feeding stations in a half circle about 3m from the hives. Foragers (> 1) were trained to accept sugar syrup at each feeding station. The foragers were permitted to collect sugar syrup at the feeding stations on the morning before the trial. When sufficient foragers were visiting the feeding stations the sugar syrup was replaced with one Petri-dish containing the jam pre-bait (control) and four dishes of either 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5% blackstrap molassesjam. The baits were removed at the end of the day and weighed to determine the amount consumed. The trial was repeated a week later with five dishes of one of the five concentrations and one control at each feeding station. The location of each concentration was different from the previous replicate. Repellancy of oxalic acid Trial 1. A large number of foragers (>5) were trained to accept sugar syrup at a feeding station. When the training solution was finished it was replaced with small dishes of 2M sucrose solutions (1 ml) containing different concentrations of oxalic acid (,.2,.5, 1, 2, and 5%). Two molar sugar syrup is considerably more attractive to honey bees than the jam baits which have a much lower sugar concentration. There were three dishes of each concentration. The dishes were arranged randomly at the feeding station. The dishes were weighed after 1

Downloaded by [6.234.11.221] at 2:16 28 February 216 Goodwin & Ten Houten-Poisoning of honey bees h to determine the amount of the solutions consumed. The trial was repeated later on the same day. Trial 2. The repellancy of the baits to inexperienced foragers was tested in the same manner as with the black strap molasses. Five feeding stations were established in a half circle about 3m from the hives. Foragers(> 1) were trained to accept sugar syrup at each feeding station. The foragers were permitted to collect sugar syrup at the feeding station in the morning before the trial. When sufficient foragers were visiting the feeding stations the sugar syrup was replaced with one petri-dish containing the jam bait (control) and five dishes of either.2,.5, 1, 2, and 5% oxalic acid in jam. The jam had been heated to 6 C to dissolve the oxalic acid. The baits were removed at the end of the day and weighed to determine the amount of jam consumed. The trial was repeated 5 days later with each concentration at a new location. Attractiveness of molasses to opossums A field trial was conducted near Maramarua State Forest (8 km NE of Hamilton). Bait stations (124) were set out on a 1.2 km track along the edge of the forest Each bait station consisted of two pieces of turf (approximately 1 m apart) turned over. A jam bait was placed on one of the turfs and a 25% blackstrap molasses-jam bait on the second turf. The number of baits consumed were counted on the second day. All the baits that were taken were then replaced with a cyanide bait to determine the type of animal that had been feeding on them. The bait stations were checked again on the following day and the number of dead animals recorded. RESULTS Exploitation of jam baits Trial 1. All the baits that were laid around the hives were completely consumed by the bees within 8 h. Trial 2. One load of the poison jam proved fatal for 62% of the foragers. The remainder appeared to recover from the early symptoms of 18 poisoning with no ill effects. A number of other bees died several hours after the start of each trial. These were presumably house bees that received 18-jam from returning foragers. The poison had a delayed action so that foragers showed no debilitating effects for up to 2 h after they had consumed the poison. The initial effects were a vigorous shaking, followed by an inability to hold onto the comb, and finally death. The earliest death (a bee lying on the floor of the hive without moving) occurred in 1.5 h. Trial 3. The delayed action of the 18 allowed foragers to make repeated trips to the poison permitting new foragers to be recruited to it Fifty five percent of the foragers made more than one trip and two were recorded making four foraging trips to a 18 bait Repellency of blackstrap molasses Trial 1. The results of presenting foragers with different concentrations of molasses can be seen in Fig. 1. When given a choice of a number of different concentrations they consistently exhibited a preference for the bait with the lowest concentration of molasses. The amount consumed decreased with increasing molasses concentrations. The small reduction in weight of the baits with high concentrations of molasses, resulted from bait sticking to the legs and bodies of the foragers that came in contact with it plus a small amount of evaporation ( < 2% ). Removal of the lowest concentration of molasses caused increased feeding activity on the next lowest concentration. By progressively removing the bait with the lowest molasses concentration it was possible to train bees to accept molasses concentrations in excess of 2%. Except when jam was presented, the foragers collected as much of the 2% molasses-jam baits when that was the lowest concentration, as they did of the 5, 1, and 15% baits, respectively when they were the lowest concentration presented. Trial 2. The proportion of the baits consumed when foragers were only given a choice of a jam bait and one concentration of molasses-jam at each feeding station can be seen in Fig. 2. In the first replicate, the jam baits were readily accepted with a sharp decline at the 1% molasses level. The sharp decline in acceptability was only evident at the 2% molasses concentration in the second replicate. The variations in amounts of bait taken between replicates were probably because of variation in the number of foragers trained to each feeding station. Repellancy of oxalic acid Trial 1. The results of the sugar syrup oxalic acid trial have been presented in Fig. 3. The foragers consumed all the,.2, and.5% oxalic acid sugar syrup within 3 min. They had, however, collected less than half the 1% oxalic acid sugar syrup by the end of the hour and less than 12% of the 2 and 5% oxalic acid sugar syrup. The results of both replicates were similar. 49

Downloaded by [6.234.11.221] at 2:16 28 February 216 5 Trial 2. The effect of presenting different concentrations of oxalic acid-jam at different feeding stations can be seen in Fig. 4. Almost all of the,.2, and.5% oxalic acid-jam was consumed, more than half of the 1%, but little of the 2 and 5% oxalic acidjam. Attractiveness of molasses baits to opossums Both baits were consumed at 74 of the 124 (59.7%) bait stations, the jam alone taken at 2 of the stations (1.6%), and only the molasses-jam baits at 4 of the stations (3.2% ). The only dead animals found the day after the cyanide baits were laid were 13 opossums. DISCUSSION From these results it can be seen that the 18 poisoning of large numbers of bees results from the delayed toxicity of 18. This permits foragers to make repeated trips to the baits allowing other foragers to be recruited. The effect is probably aggravated by the practice of laying pre-baits before the poison baits are laid. This allows foragers to become conditioned to visiting the bait stations before the poison baits are laid, increasing the possibility that the 18 baits will be found and exploited by large numbers of honey bees. The taste of the molasses-jam baits was unattractive to the bees, rather than the smell or visual aspect, as foragers landed on the baits and sampled them before leaving. Given a choice of baits with different concentrations of molasses, honey bees exhibited a preference for the bait with the lowest molasses concentration, indicating that they are able to detect small differences (5%) in molasses concentrations. The trials where the lowest concentrations were progressively removed have shown that honey bees can be trained to take baits with high molasses concentrations (2-25% ). This result was not unexpected as honey bees have been shown previously to be able to be trained to take inherently unattractive compounds as long as they are associated with a reward (von Frisch 1967). Foragers that have not been preconditioned will not readily accept concentrations of molasses in jam of 2%. However, it must be noted that some foragers consumed baits with an excess of 2% molasses. This was, however, where large numbers of honey bees had been trained to accept sugar syrup at a particular site and the syrup then replaced with molasses-jam baits. This would result in a higher degree of acceptance than normally seen under field New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 1991, Vol. 18 conditions. Also, although some of the baits with high molasses concentrations were consumed, the feeding took place within a few hours of the baits being presented. The foragers discontinued feeding on the baits before they were consumed, suggesting that littl(; would be consumed under field conditions. To reduce the acceptance of molasses-jam baits even further it is important that the molasses is added to both the poison baits and the pre-baits Uam baits) that are usually laid the day before the 18-jam baits. Failure to do this would result in foragers being trained to accept jam pre-baits which would increase the possibility of them accepting the molasses-jam 18 baits. Much higher concentrations of oxalic acid in jam baits were required to repel bees (1 %-2%) than there is in 2% blackstrap molasses (contains<.2% oxalic acid). This indicates that repellency of molasses is not just a result of its oxalic acid component but also of some other compound or compounds. Oxalic acid has the advantage over molasses in that its addition to 18-jam baits does not affect the consistency, colour, or odour of the baits. From the trials with opossums it appeared that there were no strong preferences for either jam or molasses-jam baits, suggesting that molasses could be added to opossum baits. The lack of repellency of molasses to opossums was also supported by the trials in which it was incorporated with pellets (Morgan 1985). However, subsequent cage trials have suggested that the addition of molasses or oxalic acid can reduce the amount of the baits consumed by opossums but may still be able to be added to 18- jam baits (Anon. 1989). ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This study was in part funded by the New Zealand Beekeepers Association trust funds. REFERENCES Anon. 1989: Trials seeking bee-proof 18. New Zealand farmer 111 (41): 7. Frisch, K. von 1%7: The dance language and orientation of bees. Cambridge, Harvard University Press. 566 p. Mcintosh, I. G.; Palmer-Jones, T.; Staples, E. L. J. 1964: 18 poison baits for animal pests. Wallaceville shows that proper use will not endanger bees or affect honey. The New Zealand journal of agriculture 18: 141-143. Meade, G. P.; Chen, J. C. P.1977: Cane sugar handbook, a manual for cane sugar manufacturers and their chemists. John Whiley & Sons. 945p.

Downloaded by [6.234.11.221] at 2:16 28 February 216 Goodwin & Ten Houten-Poisoning of honey bees 51 Morgan, D. R. 1985: Possum control -Making toxic baits more acceptable. Whats new in forest research 137. Palmer -Jones, T. 1957: Laboratory methods for measuring the toxicity of pesticides to honey bees. New Zealand journal of agricultural research 1: 29-3 Uematsu, H,; Sak:anoshita, A.; Seguchi, N; Suzuki, T. 1985: Foraging behaviour of honeybees on molasses. Japanese journal of applied entomology and zoology 29 (2): 97-11