The Effects of Different Doses of Caffeine on Attention and Cognitive Reaction Time among Universiti Pendidikan Sultan Idris (UPSI) Students

Similar documents
STUDY REGARDING THE RATIONALE OF COFFEE CONSUMPTION ACCORDING TO GENDER AND AGE GROUPS

THE EXPECTANCY EFFECTS OF CAFFEINE ON COGNITIVE PERFORMANCE. John E. Lothes II

2/10/2011. Caffeine and Endurance Exercise: What is best practice? Michael Leveritt. Caffeine and Endurance Exercise

L-Theanine Clinical Studies

The Effects of Caffeine on Shooting Performance

How caffeine affect college students mentality?: I-Search Research Process

Caffeine And Reaction Rates

Red Wine and Cardiovascular Disease. Does consuming red wine prevent cardiovascular disease?

International Journal of Business and Commerce Vol. 3, No.8: Apr 2014[01-10] (ISSN: )

How Much Sugar Is in Your Favorite Drinks?

Drinking caffeinated soda can give kids a burst of hyperactive energy.

Introduction. Background Information

Problem. Background & Significance 6/29/ _3_88B 1 CHD KNOWLEDGE & RISK FACTORS AMONG FILIPINO-AMERICANS CONNECTED TO PRIMARY CARE SERVICES

COMMUNICATION II Moisture Determination of Cocoa Beans by Microwave Oven

Caffeine and Theobromine Intakes of Children: Results From CSFII , 1998

Can You Tell the Difference? A Study on the Preference of Bottled Water. [Anonymous Name 1], [Anonymous Name 2]

COUNTY DETENTION COOK (Job Description)

1ACE Exercise 2. Name Date Class

Missing Data Treatments

Determination of caffeine content in tea and soft drink. BCH445 [Practical] 1

Awareness, Attitude & Usage Study Executive Summary

FACTORS ASSOCIATED WITH SOFT DRINK CONSUMPTION IN PRESCHOOL-AGED CHILDREN IN SRI LANKA.

Effect of Different Levels of Grape Pomace on Performance Broiler Chicks

Activity 3. Drink Diary (15-20 mins)

INFLUENCE OF THIN JUICE ph MANAGEMENT ON THICK JUICE COLOR IN A FACTORY UTILIZING WEAK CATION THIN JUICE SOFTENING

Emerging Local Food Systems in the Caribbean and Southern USA July 6, 2014

Plant root activity is limited to the soil bulbs Does not require technical expertise to. wetted by the water bottle emitter implement

The Grocer : Soft Drinks Research on behalf of The Grocer April 2018

Pattern and factors affecting caffeine consumption among students in a Medical University in Ajman, UAE

A NEW FEATURE EXTRACTION ALGORITHM FOR OVERLAPPING LEAVES OF RUBBER TREE SULE ANJOMSHOAE

Drink Journal PREPARATION 4-7. Sugary Drinks Extension Activity. Sugary Drinks USED BY:

Egg Rolls. Research By: Cassie Alvarado Kelsey Banks Valerie Douglass Heather Stout

Vibration Damage to Kiwifruits during Road Transportation

INVESTIGATING INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AND BRAIN ACTIVITY IN A WINE TASTING EXPERIENCE: A NEUROMARKETING APPROACH

A Comparison of X, Y, and Boomer Generation Wine Consumers in California

Laboratory Research Proposal Streusel Coffee Cake with Pureed Cannellini Beans

The impact of a continuous care intervention for treatment of type 2 diabetes on health care system utilization

Which of your fingernails comes closest to 1 cm in width? What is the length between your thumb tip and extended index finger tip? If no, why not?

COMPARISON OF EMPLOYMENT PROBLEMS OF URBANIZATION IN DISTRICT HEADQUARTERS OF HYDERABAD KARNATAKA REGION A CROSS SECTIONAL STUDY

Filtering out the facts

Running Head: MESSAGE ON A BOTTLE: THE WINE LABEL S INFLUENCE p. 1. Message on a bottle: the wine label s influence. Stephanie Marchant

Labor Supply of Married Couples in the Formal and Informal Sectors in Thailand

Biologist at Work! Experiment: Width across knuckles of: left hand. cm... right hand. cm. Analysis: Decision: /13 cm. Name

ORGANOLEPTIC EVALUATION OF RECIPES BASED ON DIFFERENT VARIETIES OF MAIZE

Please sign and date here to indicate that you have read and agree to abide by the above mentioned stipulations. Student Name #4

Materials and Methods

Step 1: Brownie batter was prepared for each oil variation following the recipe on the Betty Crocker brownie mix box.

From Selling to Supporting-Leveraging Mobile Services in the Field of Food Retailing

MacKillop Catholic College Allergy Awareness and Management Policy

Title: Western New York Sweet Corn Pheromone Trap Network Survey

What s Your Beverage?

De La Salle University Dasmariñas

Produce Education Program 2015 Evaluation Report Comparison of Key Findings

Comparative Analysis of Fresh and Dried Fish Consumption in Ondo State, Nigeria

INFLUENCES ON WINE PURCHASES: A COMPARISON BETWEEN MILLENNIALS AND PRIOR GENERATIONS. Presented to the. Faculty of the Agribusiness Department

Relationship between Mineral Nutrition and Postharvest Fruit Disorders of 'Fuerte' Avocados

Previous analysis of Syrah

PERFORMANCE OF HYBRID AND SYNTHETIC VARIETIES OF SUNFLOWER GROWN UNDER DIFFERENT LEVELS OF INPUT

2. Materials and methods. 1. Introduction. Abstract

The effects of caffeine on swimming performance in correlation with respiratory function.

Tofu is a high protein food made from soybeans that are usually sold as a block of

Thermal Properties and Temperature

NETWORK

RESULTS OF THE MARKETING SURVEY ON DRINKING BEER

FACULTY OF SCIENCE DEPARTMENT OF FOOD TECHNOLOGY (DFC) NOVEMBER EXAMINATION

WINE GRAPE TRIAL REPORT

Effects of Acai Berry on Oatmeal Cookies

DOES BEER PLAY A SOLE ROLE IN ALCOHOL AND HEALTH SYMPHONY?

The Effect of Green Tea on the Texture, Taste and Moisture of Gharidelli Double Chocolate Brownies

Consumer study on fruit - In depth interviews -

Gasoline Empirical Analysis: Competition Bureau March 2005

Y9 EXAM. Mostly on Science techniques!

Cooking Club Lesson Plan

concepts and vocabulary

Tomatoes, Lycopene and Human Health. APTRC Inc

The Path to a Longer, Healthier Life

5. Supporting documents to be provided by the applicant IMPORTANT DISCLAIMER

Health Education Lesson Plan Teacher: Grade Level: 5

Allergy Awareness and Management Policy

Handling Missing Data. Ashley Parker EDU 7312

Evaluation of Soxtec System Operating Conditions for Surface Lipid Extraction from Rice

ROAR, the University of East London Institutional Repository:

Ozone experimentation one the shelf life of various fruits

GENDER DIFFERENCES IN THE STATUS CONSUMPTION OF COFFEE

UPPER MIDWEST MARKETING AREA THE BUTTER MARKET AND BEYOND

Development and characterization of wheat breads with chestnut flour. Marta Gonzaga. Raquel Guiné Miguel Baptista Luísa Beirão-da-Costa Paula Correia

Soybean Seeding Date Effects on Productivity Jane Froese 1, Bruce Brolley 2 and Derek Lewis 1

Multiple Imputation for Missing Data in KLoSA

Candidate Number. Other Names

GENOTYPIC AND ENVIRONMENTAL EFFECTS ON BREAD-MAKING QUALITY OF WINTER WHEAT IN ROMANIA

MBA 503 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric

RELATIVE EFFICIENCY OF ESTIMATES BASED ON PERCENTAGES OF MISSINGNESS USING THREE IMPUTATION NUMBERS IN MULTIPLE IMPUTATION ANALYSIS ABSTRACT

A new approach to understand and control bitter pit in apple

FOOD ALLERGY AND MEDICAL CONDITION ACTION PLAN

(A report prepared for Milk SA)

Coffee, Health & Consumption: Reaching Coffee Drinkers

Processing Conditions on Performance of Manually Operated Tomato Slicer

KEY. Chemistry End of Year Cornerstone Assessment: Part A. Experimental Design

Year 8 Health Food Key Stage 3 Rationale September 2012 July 2013

Effects of Ground Chickpea as Wheat Flour Replacer in Corn Muffins B.A. Hollingsworth

Transcription:

56 The Effects of Different Doses of Caffeine on Attention and Cognitive Reaction Time among Universiti Pendidikan Sultan Idris (UPSI) Students Kesan Dos Kafein Berbeza terhadap Tumpuan dan Masa Reaksi Kognitif dalam kalangan pelajar Universiti Pendidikan Sultan Idris (UPSI) Normah Jusoh 1 & Niza Anira Mohd Zanani 1 1 Faculty of Sports Science and Coaching, Sultan Idris Education University, 35900 Tanjung Malim, Perak, Malaysia normah@fsskj.upsi.edu.my Abstract This study aimed to examine the effects of different doses of caffeine on attention and cognitive reaction time among UPSI students. Thirty habitual drinkers of caffeine were voluntarily participated in this study. They were assigned into three groups namely one control group (water) and two treatment groups (65 mg caffeine and 130 mg caffeine). Every group was instructed to complete the Eriksen Flanker Task after 20 minutes of drink consumption to measure attention and reaction time. The results showed that there were significant differences observed for attention (P=0.003), congruent reaction time (P=0.001) and incongruent reaction time (P=0.049) between control and treatment groups. To conclude, this study proved that caffeine could increase attention and cognitive reaction time among habitual caffeine users. Keywords Caffeine consumers, attention, reaction time Abstrak Kajian ini bertujuan menguji kesan dos yang berbeza terhadap tumpuan dan masa reaksi kognitif di kalangan pelajar UPSI. Seramai tiga puluh peserta pengguna kafein terlibat secara sukarela dalam kajian ini. Mereka dibahagikan kepada tiga kumpula iaitu satu kumpulan kawalan (air) dan dua kumpulan rawatan (65 mg dan 130 mg kafein). Setiap peserta diarahkan untuk menjawab ujian Eriksen Flanker selepas 20 minit pengambilan minuman untuk mengukur tumpuan dan fungsi kognitif. Dapatan kajian menunjukkan terdapat perbezaan yang signifikan bagi tumpuan (P=0.003), masa reaksi kongruen (P=0.001) dan masa reaksi tidak kongruen (P=0.049) antara kumpulan kawalan dan rawatan. Sebagai kesimpulan, kajian ini membuktikan bahawa kafein dapat meningkatkan tumpuan dan masa reaksi kognitif dalam kalangan pengguna kafein. Kata kunci Pengguna kafein, tumpuan, masa reaksi

57 Introduction Caffeine is a bitter substance found mainly in foods such as coffee, tea, chocolate, colas and others. Caffeine has been used widely since 1400 years ago (Roberts & Baron, 1983). For instance, the term coffee culture has been coined to show the deep impact of coffee consumption among society in most part of the world. Caffeine is primarily used as food ingredients, food supplements and added in some medications. In sports, caffeine is known as one of nutritional supplements taken by some athletes to increase sports performance. Many studies have been conducted to investigate the potential ergogenic effect of caffeine in psychomotor response (Hogervost et al., 2008; Stevenson, Hayes & Allison, 2009; Van Gelder et al., 2007), aerobic endurance performance (Del Coso et al., 2014; Schubert, Astorino & Azevedo, 2013) and muscular strength and endurance (Davis & Green, 2009; Warren, Park, Maresca, McKibans & Millard-Stafford, 2010) among athletes. These studies however produced mixed results due to different methodology applied, status of the caffeine users and dosage of caffeine used in the studies. Most athletes usually face difficulties in retaining their focus and cognitive abilities during games due to the behavior of audiences, pressure from the opponents and other environmental cues that may affect the performance (Shahruddin, 2001). There are psychological techniques available such as self-talk, imagery and others, but the use of caffeine as an ergogenic aid in sports has becoming more common to overcome such problems. Caffeine consumption stimulates the central nervous system and may affect psychomotor responses such as mental alertness and cognitive function. A study by Hogervost and colleagues (2008) showed that 100 mg caffeine could improve cognitive function during and after exercise among the well-trained cyclists. On the other hand, Hespel, Maughan and Greenhaff (2006) concluded that where a low dose of caffeine (2-5 mg/kg body mass) could potentially improve performance, however caffeine overdose should be avoided because it may impair the visual information processing in sports that require rapid visual stimuli such as soccer. Since there is no conclusive evidence about the appropriate doses of caffeine that enhances sports performance, thus this study aims to examine whether different doses of caffeine has an effect on attention and reaction time among recreationally active UPSI students. We hypothesized that the students who were given different doses of caffeine scored significantly better in attention and cognition than the control group. Methodology Participants Thirty students comprised of 15 males (aged 27±2 years; height 170±3 cm; body mass 71±9 kg) and 15 females (aged 25±1 year; height 153±8 cm; body mass 54±8 kg) were voluntarily participated in the study (Table 1). The participants were randomly selected among the recreationally active sports science students. All participants were healthy and free from chronic diseases and were habitual caffeine users. All participants received both written and verbal details of the study prior to the start of the trials. Once they agreed to proceed with the trial, written informed consent was obtained from them. This study was conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

58 Instruments and Procedures Before the trial, all participants were asked to fill in a form containing demographic information such as gender, age, weight, stature and frequency of caffeine intake per week. An Eriksen Flanker Task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974) was utilized to measure attention and cognitive function of the participants. Cognitive function was further divided into congruent and incongruent reaction time. Reliability index for the instrument was 0.80 (Miller, 1987). Thirty participants were randomly assigned to three different groups with equal numbers of both gender into the control group with no caffeine (N=5 males, 5 females), 65 mg caffeine group (N=5 males, 5 females) and 130 mg caffeine (N=5 males, 5 females). The participants were instructed not to take foods and beverages containing caffeine 30 hours before commencing the test. When the participants arrived at the laboratory, they were informed about the steps and how to answer the test. Flanker Test is represented by arrows which calculated the percentage of correct responses to measure attention. To measure cognitive function, stimuli in the form of arrows was presented one at time to the participants and the time taken to respond to the stimuli was recorded. For congruent reaction time, the participants were showed the arrows pointing in the same direction as the target stimulus (e.g. >>>>>), meanwhile incongruent reaction time was registered when the participants were presented with the arrows pointing the opposite direction to the target stimulus (e.g. << ><<). They were explained about the usage of keyboard as an instrument to response for reaction time test. The z or the / key was pressed to respond to the left or right arrow, respectively. The task consists of 36 pairs of trials with all possible combinations of stimuli. After the briefing, all participants were asked to consume their respective drink based on the experimental groups and sat quietly for 20 minutes. After that, they were instructed to answer the test. Attention score and time taken to answer the test were recorded. Data analysis All data sets were checked for normative distribution using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. As the data was parametric, MANOVA was used to evaluate the effect of independent variable (control, 65 mg caffeine and 130 mg caffeine) on the outcome variables (attention, congruent reaction time and incongruent reaction time). A Post hoc test was utilized following the significant results of the variables. The level of significant was set at P<0.05. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS version 16.0. for Windows (SPSS inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Results Table 1 shows the demographic data of the participants reported in frequency, percentage and mean ± standard deviation. In terms of frequency of caffeine intake, male participants (3.8±2.5 times per week) took caffeine more frequent compared to females (3.0±1.4 times per week).

59 Table 1 Demographic Data of the Participants (N=30) Variables Frequency (N) Percentage (%) Mean±SD Gender Age (years) Body mass (kg) Stature (cm) Frequency of caffeine intake (time/wk) 15 15 50 50 27±2 25±1 71± 9 54 ± 8 170 ± 3 153 ± 8 3.8±2.5 3.0±1.4 The findings for attention and reaction time among the three experimental groups with different doses of caffeine is shown in Table 2. There were significant differences for attention (P=0.003), congruent reaction time (P=0.001) and incongruent reaction time (P=0.049) among the participants. Table 2 Attention, Congruent and Incongruent Reaction Time among the Experimental Groups (N=30). Values in Mean±SD. Variables Control group 65 mg caffeine 130 mg caffeine P value (N=10) (N=10) (N=10) Attention score (%) 92.3±5.5** 97.5±4.9 99.5±1.6 0.003* Congruent reaction time (ms) 857.7±330.8# 541.7±87.6 478.5±89.5 0.001* Incongruent reaction time (ms) 999.7±757.2## 626.1±88.3 506.5±87.4 0.049* * Significant at P<0.05 ** Significant at P<0.05 (Bonferoni Post Hoc Test, control vs 65 mg caffeine; control vs 130 mg caffeine) # Significant at P<0.05 (Bonferoni Post Hoc Test, control vs 65 mg caffeine; control vs 130 mg caffeine) ## Significant at P<0.05 (Bonferoni Post Hoc Test, control vs 130 mg caffeine) Bonferoni Post Hoc shows that the mean attention score was significantly higher both in 130 mg caffeine group and 65 mg caffeine group compared to the control group. Likewise, both the treatment groups had significantly faster congruent reaction time than the control group. For the incongruent time reaction, 130 mg caffeine group was the fastest among the groups. The 130 mg group was two-fold faster than the control group but was not significantly different than the 65 mg group.

60 Discussion Our study documents showed that attention and cognitive function response to the different doses of caffeine given to the participants. For attention score, it was found that with the two different doses of caffeine resulted in higher percentage of focus as compared to the participants with no caffeine. The result in the present study is in agreement with the study by Rees, Allen and Lader (1999). They conducted the study on the effect of 250 mg caffeine on attention in young (20-25 years old) and elderly (50-65 years old) and found that both young and old groups who took caffeine had significantly higher attention score compared to the groups who did not take caffeine. Recent studies by Brunye, Mahoney, Lieberman and Taylor (2010) and De Bruin, Rowson, Van Buren, Rycroft and Owen (2011) also revealed the consumption of caffeine in an appropriate dose did enhance attention due to hypothetical assumption that caffeine may interact with adenosine and dopamine in the brain which stimulates the visual attention in human. In terms of cognitive function, the findings in the present study also showed that the participants who had caffeine performed better than those who did not have caffeine. Both groups who received 65 mg and 130 mg had faster congruent and incongruent reaction time than the control group. This study supports the findings by Smit and Rogers (2000) who concluded that caffeine acts as a stimulant psychologically and physically. They found that caffeine doses at 12.5 mg until 100 mg had a positive effect on reaction time, with higher doses resulted in faster reaction time. Besides, a study by Mihai, Gernot and Mihaela (2011) also showed that the university students who took caffeine in Nescafe and energy drink had better cognitive function than those who did not. Indeed, Smith, Christopher and Sunderland (2013) showed that the caffeine consumers who were withdrawn from caffeine for few days still had faster reaction time and better cognitive function than those who were non-consumer of caffeine. Even though many studies have proven the positive effect of caffeine on attention and cognitive function, there are other studies which showed otherwise. For example, Yeomans, Javaherian, Thovey and Stafford (2005) examined the effect of caffeine on cognitive function in caffeine drinkers versus non-consumer. The results revealed that the non-consumers had faster reaction time than the caffeine consumers who had overnight caffeine abstinence. In the study, the authors hypothesized that the degradation of cognition following caffeine withdrawal was due to the fall in caffeine concentration below the threshold level of cognitive response. In our study, all participants were habitual caffeine drinkers who were asked not to take caffeine the day before the trials. This may explain the findings in our study which showed that the participants who were not given any caffeine (control group) had a significantly two-fold decrease in incongruent reaction time compared to the 130 mg experimental group. Our study also demonstrated that there was no significant difference in attention score and reaction time between the doses of 65 mg and 130 mg caffeine. In the present study, all participants were tested 20 minutes after ingesting the drinks, which is the average time taken for caffeine to produce effects on cognitive function (Mumford et al., 1994). We could not find the difference between two doses of caffeine probably because the dose may produce an effect at a different time after consumption. According to Smit and Rogers

61 (2000), low caffeine dose started to show its effect on cognitive performance after 40 minutes of consumption. Similarly, Heatherly, Hayward, Seers and Rogers (2005) proved that the longer the absorption rates of caffeine, the stronger the effect on the cognitive function. In their study, no difference was found at the 4 hours, but the significant different on cognitive performance was observed at the 6 hours and 8 hours between the control group and the treatment group who received 1.2 mg/kg body mass. Conclusion The doses of 65 mg and 130 mg caffeine produced significant effects on attention and cognitive reaction time among habitual caffeine users. The doses are equivalent to one and two glasses of coffee intake, respectively. Given the fact that caffeine is a diuretic which induces body fluid loss, however, our study concluded that an appropriate dose may confer some benefits in term of improving mental alertness and cognitive function. Further studies are warranted to look into the effect of higher dose of caffeine on physiological and psychological changes in all populations because caffeine intake particularly coffee consumption worldwide is on the hike every year. Acknowledgement We are grateful to the staff at the Faculty of Sport Science and Coaching for helping with the data collection. We also thank the Faculty for allowing us to use the computers to run the Flanker Task. References Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., Lieberman, H. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). Caffeine modulates attention network function. Brain and Cognition, 72, 181-188. Davis, J. K and Green, J. M. (2009). Caffeine and anaerobic performance: ergogenic value and mechanisms of action. Sports Medicine, 39(10), 813-832. De Bruin, E. A., Rowson, M. J., Van Buren, L., Rycroft, J. A., & Owen, G. N. (2011). Black tea improves attention and self-reported alertness. Appetite, 56, 235-240. Del Coso, J., Perez-Lopez, A., Abian-Vicen, J., Salinero, J. J., Lara, B and Valades, D. (2014). Enhancing physical performance in male volleyball players with a caffeine- containing energy drink. International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance, 9(6), 1013-1018. Eriksen, B. A and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143-149. Heatherly, V. S., Hayward, C. R., Seers, E. H and Rogers, J. P. (2005). Cognitive and psychomotor performance, mood, and pressor effects on caffeine after 4, 6, and 8 h caffeine abstinence. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 178(4), 461-470. Hespel, P., Maughan, R. J and Greenhaff, P. L. (2006). Dietary supplements for football. Journal of Sports Science, 24(7), 749-761. Hogervost, E., Bandelow, S., Schmitt, J., Jentjens, R., Oliveira, M., Carter, J and Gleeson, M. (2008). Caffeine improves physical and cognitive performance during exhaustive exercise. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 40(10), 1841-1851.

62 Mihai, A., Gernot, S and Mihaela, C. (2011). The influence of energy drinks and caffeine on reaction time and cognitive processes in young Romanian students. Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 30, 662-670. Miller, J. (1987). Priming is not necessary for selective-attention failures: semantic effects of unattended, unprimed letters. Perception and Psychophysics, 41, 419-434. Mumford, G. K., Benowitz, N. L., Evans, S. M., Kamisnki, B. J., Preston, K. L., Sannerud, C. A., & Griffins, R. R. (1994). Absorption rate of methylxanthines following capsules, cola and chocolate. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 51, 319-325. Rees, K., Allen, D and Lader, M. (1999). The influence of age on psychomotor and cognitive function. Psychopharmacology(Berl), 145(2), 181-188. Roberts, H. R and Baron, J. J. (1983). Biological effect of caffeine: History and Use. Journal of Food Technology, 37, 32-39. Schubert, M. M., Astorino, T. A and Azevedo, J. L. (2013). The effects of caffeinated energy shots on time trial performance. Nutrients, 6, 2062-2075. Shahruddin, Abd Aziz (2001). Mengaplikasi teori psikologi dalam sukan. Kuala Lumpur: Utusan Publication & Distribution Sdn. Bhd. Smit, H. J and Rogers, P. J. (2000). Effect of low doses of caffeine on cognitive performance, mood and thirst in low and higher caffeine consumers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 152, 167-173. Smith, A. P., Christopher, G and Sunderland, D. (2013). Acute effects of caffeine on attention: a comparison of non-consumers and withdrawn consumers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 27(1), 77-83. Stevenson, E. J., Hayes, P. R and Allison, S. J. (2009). The effect of a carbohydrate-caffeine sports drink on simulated golf performance. Applied Physiology, Nutrition and Metabolism, 34(4), 681-688. Van Gelder, B. M., Buijsse, B., Tijhuis, M., Kalmijn, S., Giampaoli, S., Nissinen, A and Kromhout, D. (2007). Coffee consumption is inversely associated with cognitive decline in elderly European men: the FINE study. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 61, 226-232. Warren, G. L., Park, N. D., Maresca, R. D., McKibans, K. I and Millard-Stafford, M. L.(2010). Effect of caffeine ingestion on muscular strength and endurance: a meta-analysis. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 42(7), 1375-1387. Yeomans, M. R., Javaherian, S., Thovey, M. H and Stafford, D. L. (2005). Attentional bias for caffeinerelated stimuli in high but not moderate or non-caffeine consumers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 181(3), 477-485.